Friday, July 8, 2016

What You See Isn’t Always What You Get: The Eyewitness Problem

     If our memories were perfect, we would never need to study, we would never have to prepare grocery lists or write down someone’s phone number. Unfortunately, these are activities that everyone does constantly throughout their lives. It comes as no surprise that when the average person is asked to remember specific details from a past event, their memory falls slightly short of the truth. Eyewitnesses are important and can provide details to a case that would otherwise remain undiscovered, however; it is important to keep in mind that the human memory is not perfect. Even worse, if we suffer gaps in our memory, we tend to fill that empty space with details that make sense to us, but may not be true.

     In a New York Law Journal article written by Florina Altshiler, she discusses the pros and cons that Eyewitnesses bring to a trial. The article brings up several key points that describe the situations eyewitnesses find themselves in and how their memory is affected by each.

     First, consider the potential cases in which an eyewitness are considered to be valuable. Cases involving car accidents, shootings, assaults, or robberies may occur quickly and under extreme stress. A person witnessing such events must process a lot of information within just a few minutes, sometimes seconds! Now think of memory as it is formed. Every person has their own opinions, biases, and ideas of how things should be. This does have an effect on how we remember things. Even a person’s mood has an effect on their willingness and ability to remember parts of their day.

     Here’s an example: Kelly is on her way to return a defective curtain rod she had purchased from a store in downtown Potsdam, New York. She is doing this during her lunch hour and because of this, there are many noisy vehicles on the street. Kelly is frustrated at the poor quality of the curtain rod and is paying little attention to those around her. She is also in a hurry because her lunch hour ends in the next twenty minutes. As she is walking down the street, two men run out from the alleyway in front of her. The first man is wearing a bright blue hoodie and is bleeding from his left shoulder. The man chasing him is wearing a bright purple hoodie and is carrying a rusty pipe in his right hand. The first man has a beard while the second is clean-shaven. Both are wearing hats. The two men turn the corner and are out of sight in the next five seconds. Kelly has just become an eyewitness to a future case. She will be asked to recall specific details from five seconds of her busy day. Kelly may have a good memory, but her mind did not have time to processes the chase. She may mix up the men’s clothing colors or mistake the rusty pipe for a baseball bat. When the incident occurred, Kelly’s mind was focused on other areas of her life. Kelly may present some valuable evidence at the trial, but then again, she may remember the specifics differently.

     The testimony of an eyewitness can be skewed in other ways as well. Police officers who interview eyewitnesses may ask suggestive questions, or questions that suggest details of a crime to a witness rather than allow the witness to remember them on their own. These suggestive questions may not be intentional; however, they do change an eyewitness’s perception all the same. It is important for members of law enforcement to ask open-ended questions that encourage the eyewitness to remember the crime without specific prompting. There is also research to show that when there are weapons present, such as a gun or a knife, witnesses tend to focus their attention on those objects rather than on the crime itself.
     Eyewitnesses might also face a group mentality when recalling events if the incident was seen by many people. To fill in memory gaps, eyewitnesses may rely on the memory of another to work out the details. This patchwork memory may not be correct, but if the majority of witnesses all agree that something did happen a certain way, it is difficult for a single witness to argue against it without hard proof.

     Jury members should take into account the flaws of human memory when deciding the outcome of a trial. Eyewitness testimony should never be dismissed or disregarded as unimportant, however; if harder evidence exists such as surveillance footage, it should be regarded with greater attention. It is also unwise for those in law enforcement or private investigators to lead a witness with questions that may alter their memory of an incident. Lastly, eyewitnesses themselves should not force memories to form. As noted in the article, memories are temporary creations that we create for ourselves. Eyewitnesses should describe events to the best of their abilities, but should not be pressured into creating false memories.

     For more information concerning eyewitness reports or how an eyewitness is considered in the court room, contact Poissant, Nichols, Grue, and Vanier, P.C. at:

367 West Main Street                                                         45 Market Street
Malone, NY 12953                                                       Potsdam, NY 13676

Phone: (518) 483-1440
Toll Free: 1-800-924-3529



-Paul Nichols 

No comments:

Post a Comment